-->
Educators remain almost entirely baffled by the role played
by social interaction in learning; fond as they are of the ‘knowledge transfer’
model of learning in which – it seems – the instructor should only have to
speak in order for the student to learn. I thought it might be helpful to contrast
two learning scenarios to highlight the underlying issues.
Scenario 1:
An Instructor displays a slide titled ‘characteristics
of a good leader’ on a screen, for a room full of participants. The slide
contains a number of bullet-pointed items including the words ‘provides recognition’,
‘structures development’ and ‘flexes style’. The instructor reads each item and
elaborates:
Instructor: So these are just some of the things you’d
expect of a good leader – they’re not in any particular order… obviously it’s
important for leaders to recognise the achievements of people in their team –
not necessarily formal recognition – just, you know, words of encouragement. I
say it’s obvious but often leaders forget… there can be so much to do… but a
little recognition goes a long way. ‘Structures development’ – as a leader you
will need to think about staff development… how you are going to build
capability – and you might think about future needs – aligning with the
business strategy, that sort of thing. ‘Flexes style’ – meaning that leaders
all have a tendency to lead in certain ways – but, you know, each person in the
team is an individual and will have individual needs – so leaders should be
able to accommodate different needs…
Analysis:
this approach is likely to result in low levels of
recall. None of the words is intrinsically distinctive (i.e. causing a significant
affective response). As an example, the words ‘death by electrocution’ would
likely be sufficient to be memorable in their own right – whereas the words ‘structures
development’ are commonplace – indeed ‘development’ is likely a euphemism for ‘education’
which for many participants may have mildly connotations of boredom and pointlessness.
Equally there are no distinctive images on the slide likely to cause an
affective response, so no visual cues for the content that may be recalled
later (as with words, visual elements are analysed according to their affective
elements and the affective responses stored). This is an utterly forgettable slide, in other words.
The instructor makes no attempt at storytelling.
Storytelling is a format designed to add affective significance to information,
thereby rendering it memorable. The words by themselves have little significance and as such are not stored. The use of repetitive, bland and abstract
language actually serves to deepen their forget-ability – neither the words or the
delivery are such that they would cause much reaction in the participants’
resulting in little or no likelihood of recall.
Participants are not involved in any ostensible way. There
is nothing personally ‘at stake’ for the participants – for example they are not expecting
to be challenged to explain what has been said at any point, or to contribute
their own stories, or to compete – hence the material has no contextual
affective significance: it is merely sounds.
The instructor has not actively encouraged participation;
social norms for this situation strongly suggest to participants that all that
is required of them is that they give the impression of attention and not
interrupt unless called upon to do so.
Since participants are not contributing, the instructor's content is not
explicitly a reaction to questions or concerns they have formed in their mind.
By contrast, content which reinforces existing beliefs/behaviours (e.g. the
belief that people should be thanked on a daily basis) will result in a
positive reaction – hence be more likely to be encoded. By minimising
participation, the content is not delivered as confirmation/disconfirmation of
participants’ concerns and therefore causes little reaction.
Scenario 2:
An instructor suggests that participants create
a life-size drawing of a ‘good leader’ using their creative imagination to
illustrate the drawing with features they feel reflect desired characteristics.
Instructor: so I’ve provided some paper and pens – you’ve
got 20 minutes and then I would like to hear from you.
P1: Wow. Ok. I wasn’t expecting that!
P2: Who’s good at drawing then? (smiles)
P3: Not me – I’m rubbish – I got a ‘D’ in Art at school!
(everyone laughs)
P4: Go on then – I’ll give it a go! (takes pen)
P2: Oh! Looks like we’ve got an artist among us! (laughs)
Instructor: That’s great – but I’d like everyone to make a
contribution!
P3: thought I was going to get away with it then! (laughs)
…
P1: I had a brilliant manager – always encouraging me… that
was the best team I worked for…
P2: How can we draw that?
P4: How about pom-poms – like a ‘cheerleader’? (everyone
laughs)
P2: Yeah – draw that then!
P4: How about you draw it? (hands pen to P3)
P3: Oh, all right then (laughs, draws pom-poms)
…
Instructor: ok, so why is your drawing holding a sponge?
(everyone laughs)
P4: It’s pom-poms – like a cheerleader – ‘cos they encourage
people.
Instructor: great! what impact does that have?
P1: well I was saying that I had a brilliant manager when I
was working in procurement – I mean the job was pretty dull – but the way she
did it – she wrote me a thank-you note for this one project – I just thought it
was really nice – I still have it!
Instructor: hands-up who has ever written a thank-you note
for someone in their teams?
(some hands go up)
Instructor: what about this here – is that an axe!?
(everyone laughs)
P2: no – that’s just my drawing – it’s a signpost…
Instructor: Ah – I get it. What are they signposting?
P2: well – a good leader should identify opportunities… be
there to coach people – basically help people with their career…
Instructor: good point. Has anyone experienced a manager
like that?
P3: I have.
Instructor: what difference did it make?
P3: big difference. I don’t think I would be in this job
without it.
…
Analysis:
The participants are immediately, visibly excited.
This excitement already ensures that the activity is memorable whatever the
outcome. There are a number of reasons why participants are likely to be
excited: some of the other participants they have not met, and social
interaction offers the prospect of affective rewards – validation and
relationship-formation to name two. The activity presents participants with the
opportunity to present themselves favourably – as self-deprecating, for
example, or as talented. The unusual nature of the activity ensures that it
creates the strong affective response that characterises novel stimuli.
Participants vocalise this reaction immediately – their shared reaction assisting
in the formation of a minimal group.
Participants immediately set about seeking validation –
eliciting a variety of positive reactions from the new group, from laughter to
admiration. Their emotional state is heightened by the relatively significant social
stakes of being introduced to a new group of people. Behaviours and
vocalisations are aimed at eliciting positive feedback – behaviours such as
storytelling, joking, drawing – all over which will result in a bump in self
esteem if accomplished correctly.
The instructor ensures that each individual participates,
since it is possible that some people will elect to ‘opt-out’ of the
interaction – and this is likely to reduce recall for the experience and
related information, since nothing of personal significance was ventured.
Participant 1 engages in simple storytelling – this is
almost always seen as a positive contribution to a group, as stories are
vehicles for important information and a way in which group member can
therefore demonstrate altruism. The creative challenges necessitate deeper
processing. The implicitly competitive nature of the interaction deepens the
affective significance (no-one wants to come up with a ‘stupid’ idea). The ‘pom-pom’
idea is both distinctive enough and unusual enough to warrant a round of
laughter – in addition the visual imagery is unusual and hence liable to result
in improved recall.
The poor execution of the drawing actually adds to the
memorability of the exercise. The participant challenged over the ‘sponge’
interpretation feels a sense of personal responsibility and shame – but one
which plays into their overall opportunity to position themselves as
self-deprecating. By challenging the significance of this item, the instructor
is able to elicit further stories from the group. These stories greatly enhance
the memorability for the information, for both the storyteller and the
receiver. Telling a story for a group of strangers is always a ‘high stakes’
activity socially, and stories convey valuable social information that is
typically related to practical application.
The bizarre illustrations - however executed - are strong memory cues, since they involve elements of personal accountability, they are linked to an exciting episode or memorable stories. Participants have no difficulty later recalling the exercise and some of the elements of the sketch - which in turn act as cues for additional information, such as the significance of a behaviour as illustrated by a story.
The bizarre illustrations - however executed - are strong memory cues, since they involve elements of personal accountability, they are linked to an exciting episode or memorable stories. Participants have no difficulty later recalling the exercise and some of the elements of the sketch - which in turn act as cues for additional information, such as the significance of a behaviour as illustrated by a story.
By challenging the group to give account of their own
behaviours, the instructor presents a participants with a dilemma – they must
quickly decide how raising or lowering their hand will impact their image
amongst the group, and whether or not lying is a viable or desirable challenge.
Consequently this leaves a lasting affective trace.
Conclusion:
The purpose of this ‘micro-analysis’ is not necessarily to
advocate for the latter approach, but merely to point out how crude is our
current understanding of what is really going on when people learn – and how
much progress we can make when we have a grasp of the basics.
Social Learning approaches such as that proposed by Vygotsky
are lamentably primitive in enabling us to understand the role played by social
interaction in learning. Certainly it can help to mix more capable people with
those who are less capable – but this only hints at the affective mechanics
that underpins the interaction: what is the relationship between these people? What
do they care about? What do they react to?
Whether or not you agree with the specifics of the analysis
in the second example, it is indubitable that there is a veritable blizzard of
subtle social (and therefore affective) activity taking place in the latter
condition, which is largely absent in the former. If we accept the central premise
that learning is a consequence of our affective responses to experiences, then we
can immediately see that the two situations are radically different from a
learning standpoint.
Neither am I suggesting that the latter example is a model
of learning design – indeed there are some major causes for concern: whilst I
have no doubt that the latter results in a more memorable experience - and one that
is more likely to be recalled – it is not clear to what extent this learning
will transfer into actual behaviour. Merely creating an exciting
socially-charged activity in the interests of boosting recall cannot be
sufficient; probably we would do better to marry this social dimension to an
activity that more closely resembles the intended performance – rather than the
somewhat tokenistic activity of making a sketch. Improved learning results might also be accounted for by the Levels of Processing model which predicts better recall as a result of deeper processing - so I am not suggesting this illustration constitutes experimental support for the Affective Context theory - just that there is a very different way of thinking about what is going on in these situations.
In sum, it is not enough for us to criticise the lecture format
without being able to say precisely what is wrong with it – after all, I can
learn perfectly well from a book if I choose to. I hope this post highlights
the role played by the affective context in determining what we learn, whatever the situation.
No comments:
Post a Comment